Kaila Bruer

 KailaC. Bruer

Kaila C. Bruer

  • Courses4
  • Reviews16

Biography

University of Regina - Psychology


Resume

  • 2019

    University of Regina

    Royal Canadian Mounted Police

    University of Regina

    Research Specialist

    Farm Credit Canada

    Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

    University of Toronto

    Toronto

    Canada Area

    Postdoctorate Fellow

    Regina

    SK

    Doctoral Student

    University of Regina

    University of Regina

    Social Psychology - PSYC. 220

    Sessional Lecturer

    $20

    000/year for up to 4 year award to support doctoral studies (declined).

    Social Science and Humanities Research Council

    Ontario Graduate Scholarship

    $15

    000 award to support students in a Masters program

    Ontario Provincial Government

  • 2013

    Doctor of Philosophy (PhD)

    Experimental and Applied Psychology

    University of Regina

  • 2012

    Royal Ottawa Mental Health Centre

    Royal Ottawa Mental Health Centre

    Teaching Assistant

    Introduction to Psychology PSYC 101\nAbnormal Psychology PSYC 333\nForensic Psychology PSYC 321

    University of Regina

  • 2010

    Carleton University

    Farm Credit Canada

    University of Regina

    Research Assistant

    Ottawa

    Canada Area

    Royal Canadian Mounted Police

    Carleton University

    University of Regina

    Forensic Psychology - PSYC. 321

    University Teaching Fellow

    Master of Arts (M.A.)

    Forensic Psychology

    Carleton University

  • 2006

    Bachelor of Arts (BA)

    Criminology and Criminal Justice

    Carleton University

  • 3

    The purpose of this research was to examine the influence of eyewitness age and familiarity with the defendant

    and the potential interaction between these variables

    on mock jurors' perceptions and decisions. Mock jurors were presented with a mock murder trial transcript that included a positive identification by the victim's daughter who was 4

    or 20 years old and saw the defendant 0

    or 6 times prior to the crime. It was hypothesized that when a witness was familiar with the defendant

    their testimony would be perceived to be more credible and would sway verdict decisions even in the case of a very young eyewitness. Mock jurors were significantly more likely to rate the 20-year-old eyewitness as more credible overall and

    specifically

    in her description of the crime than the 4-year-old eyewitness; however

    age of the eyewitness did not significantly influence mock jurors' verdict decisions. Witness familiarity with the defendant did not significantly influence mock jurors' perceptions or verdicts and an interaction between eyewitness age and familiarity was not found. The implications of these findings will be discussed.

    Eyewitness age and familiarity with the defendant: Influential factors in mock jurors' assessment of guilt

    Judicial Discussion of Eyewitness Identification Evidence

    To safeguard against unreliable eyewitness evidence in court

    judges must be aware of recommendations\nput forth by empirical researchers and legal inquiries (such as the Sophonow Inquiry; Cory

    2001).\nHowever

    we do not know how judges weigh and assess the reliability of eyewitness identification\nevidence in their decisions. 247 full-text reports of Canadian judicial trial and appeal decisions from 1980\nto 2016 involving a discussion of eyewitness identification evidence were coded for key system (e.g.

    \nlineup administration) and estimator variables (e.g.

    experiencing trauma) known to impact eyewitness\nreliability. Judicial discussion of these variables was infrequent; however

    judicial decision was sensitive\nto information available about how a lineup was administered as well as whether the eyewitness was\nperceived to have experienced trauma during the crime. Lack of consideration of other key variables\nduring decision-making may indicate a potential lack of subject-area knowledge as well as possible\njudicial use of heuristics that minimise the concern about eyewitness evidence reliability in the case at\nhand.

    Judicial Discussion of Eyewitness Identification Evidence

    Ryan J. Fitzgerald

    Visually salient line-up rejection options have not been systematically studied with adult eyewitnesses. We explored the impact of using a non-verbal

    salient rejection option on adults' identification accuracy for line-ups containing low- or high-similarity fillers. The non-verbal

    salient rejection option had minimal impact on accuracy in low-similarity line-ups

    but in high-similarity line-ups its inclusion increased correct rejections for target-absent line-ups as well as incorrect rejections in target-present line-ups

    relative to a verbal rejection condition. The improved performance in target-absent line-ups suggests that adults

    like children

    may experience pressure to choose and guess during difficult tasks. This pressure is reduced when a prominent non-verbal rejection option is displayed in the line-up. However

    the salient rejection option also appears to increase the attractiveness of avoiding a difficult choice between the target and highly similar fillers. Implications of these findings for the experimental literature and justice system are discussed.

    Lineup member similarity influences the effectiveness of a salient rejection option for eyewitnesses.

    Leora Dahl

    Laypersons were asked to assume the role of investigators to explore judgments of what evidence is needed to make an arrest in a criminal investigation when an alibi witness is present. Participants were sensitive to the relationship between the alibi witness and the suspect and were more likely to believe an alibi provided by someone unrelated to the suspect

    as evidenced by requests for more physical evidence against the suspect than when the alibi corroborator was a family member. In addition

    when presented with contradictory evidence

    the age of the alibi witness became an important consideration. Age alone did not impact perceptions of evidence adequacy; however

    when an (adult) eyewitness provided testimony that contradicted a child alibi witness

    participants demonstrated partiality towards believing the child as evidenced by (a) more requests for physical evidence to be convinced the child was wrong and to arrest the suspect and (b) higher ratings of alibi witness credibility. This effect was not seen when the eyewitness’s testimony contradicted an alibi provided by an adult. The results provide insight for investigators and legal counsel regarding the influence of varying types of alibi witness evidence.

    When an alibi is not enough: An exploration of evidence needed for mock investigators to lay charges in a robbery case

    The purpose of this research was to determine if child eyewitnesses are seen as more or less credible compared with older eyewitnesses and to determine whether the number of descriptive errors made while recalling the appearance of a perpetrator has an influence on perceived credibility of the witness. Mock jurors were given a mock trial that presented a positive identification by an eyewitness where age of the eyewitness (4-

    12-

    20-year-old) and the number of perpetrator descriptor errors (i.e.

    6) made by the eyewitness were manipulated. Perceived levels of credibility

    accuracy

    and determinations of guilt were compared using a self-report questionnaire. Results support the hypothesis that mock jurors perceive eyewitnesses who make fewer errors in descriptions with more integrity (i.e.

    more credible

    reliable

    and accurate) and perceive the evidence presented by them (i.e.

    description of perpetrator and description of events) as more reliable. Overall

    adult eyewitnesses are perceived with more integrity\nthan child eyewitnesses.

    Influence of eyewitness age and recall error on mock juror decision-making

    Kaila

    Bruer

    Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

    University of Toronto

  • 000

    Joseph-Armand Bombardier Canadian Graduate Scholarship

    Doctoral Scholarships: $105

    Social Science and Humanities Research Council

  • Research

    Research Design

    Report Writing

    Forensic Psychology

    forensic psychology

    Quantitative Research

    Government Reports

    Criminology

    research

    University Teaching

    Public Speaking

    clinical research

    marketing research

    Clinical Research

    academic writing

    policy

    government reports

    Policy

    policy research

    Academic Writing

    The “magical” effect of integration on autobiographical memory.

    Tom Phenix

    Only a handful of studies have extended our understanding of retrieval induced forgetting (RIF) during development and even fewer have tested for RIF effects outside word-list paradigms. The purpose of these experiments was to: (i) examine how partial retrieval of a witnessed event would impact subsequent retrieval of that event in school-aged children; and (ii) examine the robustness of semantic integration as a boundary condition on RIF. Two experiments were conducted using the three traditional phases of the RIF paradigm: study phase

    practice phase

    and test phase. We found clear evidence of RIF in event memory. There was also evidence of the robust impact that integration instructions have on minimizing RIF. Integration appears to not only have a dampening effect on RIF

    but integration instructions may also influence how children process all aspects of an experience

    regardless of whether a person is passively or actively part of the experience.

    The “magical” effect of integration on autobiographical memory.

    Child eyewitnesses make more mistakes than adults when identifying criminals. A new police lineup design could help us assess their reliability and prevent wrongful convictions.

    Helping child witnesses: A new design for police lineups

    \nLaypersons were asked to assume the role of investigators to explore judgments of what evidence is needed to make an arrest in a criminal investigation when an alibi witness is present. Participants were sensitive to the relationship between the alibi witness and the suspect and were more likely to believe an alibi provided by someone unrelated to the suspect

    as evidenced by requests for more physical evidence against the suspect than when the alibi corroborator was a family member. In addition

    when presented with contradictory evidence

    the age of the alibi witness became an important consideration. Age alone did not impact perceptions of evidence adequacy; however

    when an (adult) eyewitness provided testimony that contradicted a child alibi witness

    participants demonstrated partiality towards believing the child as evidenced by (a) more requests for physical evidence to be convinced the child was wrong and to arrest the suspect and (b) higher ratings of alibi witness credibility. This effect was not seen when the eyewitness’s testimony contradicted an alibi provided by an adult. The results provide insight for investigators and legal counsel regarding the influence of varying types of alibi witness evidence.

    When an alibi is not enough: An exploration of evidence needed for mock investigators to lay charges in a robbery case

    The purpose of this study was to assess young children’s lineup identification performance comparative to adults and to determine whether developmental variability exists in reaction time when making correct and incorrect identification decisions across target-present and target-absent lineups. Adults (M_age = 20.00) and young children (M_age= 4.69) were exposed to an unfamiliar target and the time taken for them to make a lineup decision was automatically recorded by touching the picture on a computer screen. Children were found to have fewer hits (i.e.

    correct identifications and rejections) and more false alarms than adults in both lineup types. In addition

    results support the hypothesis that a faster decision was related to improved accuracy for adults

    while a slower decision may be suggestive of reduced accuracy for children. The developmental variability may suggest that developmental cognitive factors may influence lineup behaviour.

    Does Speed Indicate Lineup Identification Accuracy? Examining Children's and Adults' Reaction Time.

    To date

    very little research has examined developmental reversals in false memory outside of the word list paradigm. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether knowledge of

    and familiarity with types of environments influenced the quantity

    accuracy

    and errors recalled by children (4- to 6- year-olds) and adults. Using images and open recall questions

    the findings of the present study support the predicted reversal in false memories (i.e.

    developmental variability)

    supporting a cross-over effect of age and false memories. Children performed better than adults through providing less relevant errors (i.e.

    commission or semantic-based errors) about environments that were more familiar to adults. These findings support the predicted developmental reversal in false memories. The findings are discussed within the context of fuzzy trace theory and semantic knowledge structures. Implications for the applied eyewitness context are examined.

    Familiarity and Recall Memory for Environments: A comparison of Children and Adults

    In two experiments and one follow-up analysis

    we examined the impact of using a repeated forced-choice (RFC) line-up procedure with child and adult eyewitnesses. The RFC procedure divides the identification task into a series of exhaustive binary comparisons that produces not only traditional line-up information (identification decision and confidence) but also information about witness' selection behavior. Experiment 1 revealed that younger children (6- to 8-year-olds) struggled with the RFC procedure

    while older children (9- to 11-year-olds) performed as well with the RFC procedure as with a simultaneous procedure (with wildcard). Experiment 2 replicated this comparable performance with adults. Witnesses' suspect selection behavior during the RFC was predictive of identification accuracy for older children and adults. A model examined the additional information provided by the RFC in experiments 1 and 2 and provided evidence that witnesses' patterns of responding can be used to estimate suspect selection bias (a proxy for suspect recognition strength) associated with individual line-up decisions.

    A repeated forced-choice lineup procedure provides suspect bias information with no cost to accuracy for older children and adults.

    We tested whether an alternative lineup procedure designed to minimize problematic influences (e.g.

    metacognitive development) on decision criteria could be effectively used by children and improve child eyewitness identification performance relative to a standard identification task. Five hundred sixteen children (6- to 13-year-olds) watched a video of a target reading word lists and

    the next day

    made confidence ratings for each lineup member or standard categorical decisions for 8 lineup members presented sequentially. Two algorithms were applied to classify confidence ratings into categorical decisions and facilitate comparisons across conditions. The classification algorithms produced accuracy rates for the confidence rating procedure that were comparable to the categorical procedure. These findings demonstrate that children can use a ratings-based procedure to discriminate between previously seen and unseen faces. In turn

    this invites more nuanced and empirical consideration of ratings-based identification evidence as a probabilistic index of guilt that may attenuate problematic social influences on child witnesses’ decision criteria.

    How sure are you that this is the man you saw? Using confidence judgments to identify a target with child eyewitnesses.

    Julie Dempsey

    Joanna D. Pozzulo

    Young children (4- to 7-years old; N = 59) and adults (N = 53) were shown a series of targets that were either familiar (i.e.

    popular cartoon characters) or unfamiliar (unknown human faces) to assess whether children’s false positive responding with target-absent lineups is driven by social factors to a greater degree than cognitive factors. Although children were able to produce correct identification rates with virtually 100% accuracy for the cartoon characters

    they produced a significantly lower correct rejection rate compared to adults. Children also produced a significantly lower correct rejection rate for the human faces compared to adults. These data are discussed for understanding children’s identification evidence.

    The Culprit in Target-Absent Lineups: Understanding Young Children’s False Positive Responding

    Saria Javaid

    Shannon Parsons

    Jennifer Pettalia

PSYC 321

2(11)

PSYC 102

4.3(3)